Week 5 practical

Using trial data for contingent trials, saving data to the server

The plan for week 5 practical

This week we are going to look at a bit more of the Online Experiments with jsPsych tutorial, and then look at code for a simple word learning / frequency learning experiment based on the Ferdinand et al. (2019) paper we read this week. Our implementation of this experiment builds on the self-paced reading experiment in that it uses nested timelines and functions to construct trials which have a fairly complex structure (although you can implement it more simply than that). It also requires randomisation and contingent trials (what the participant sees on one trial depends on what they did at the previous trial), so we need to introduce some infrastructure to do that. Finally, I’ll add some code to save data on the server at the end of the experiment, rather than just dumping it to the screen. Note that saving the data at the end of the experiment is not the best way to do it and we do not recommend this for projects involving real data collection, e.g. your dissertation! See the note on that below.

There is quite a lot of new technical content this week, next week will be less intense! Remember, the idea is that you can work through these practicals in the lab classes and, if necessary, in your own time (e.g. if you want to make a start before the lab class, or if you don’t complete the practical in the lab class) - the lab class provides dedicated time each week to focus on doing the practicals with on-tap support from the teaching team, but you may need more than the 2 hours to get these practicals done. We are happy to help with the previous week’s class if you tried to finish it off in your own time and need some help.

Tutorial content

Read through section 06 of the Online Experiments with jsPsych tutorial - you will see the technique in the “Sending the data line by line” section next week, but no harm reading about it now. You don’t need to do the exercises dotted through that tutorial, since we will see the same bits of code in the word learning experiment. The key things you need to take away from the tutorial are:

A word learning experiment

First: you build it!

As with last week, we’d like to give you an opportunity to try to build (parts of) this word-learning experiment yourself. As with last week, to help you get started, we are going to provide you with a couple of templates to fill in - an html file that loads some of the plugins you will need (but once you decide what additional plugins you need you will have to load them too, e.g. by copying the appropriate script line from the plugin documentation), then a javascript file where you can put your own code. That javascript file includes some extra stuff (instructions, saving data to the server) that we pre-built for you so you can focus on the more interesting parts of the experiment.

You are actually going to need a bunch of files for this experiment - the html and javascript templates for your attempt (which we are calling my_word_learning.html and my_word_learning.js), the html and js file for our implementation (word_learning.html and word_learning.js), but also a utilities.js file (which includes the save_data function), a php file (for saving data) and a folder containing a bunch of images. Rather than downloading these separately, download the following zip file and then uncompress it into your usual jsPsych folder, in a folder called something like word_learning, alongside your grammaticality_judgments and self_paced_reading folders. :

This code should run on your local computer, but you should upload the whole word_learning folder to the public_html folder on the jspsychlearning server and play with it there. Note that the code that saves the data to the server will only work if your code is actually running on the jspsychlearning server - if you are running it on your own computer the data will not save anywhere, although it will still be shown on-screen. This is because your personal computer isn’t running anything that can handle POST commands and process them with PHP, which is what is involved in saving data - those things are all set up on the jspsychlearning server for you. So if you haven’t already got to grips with putting your code on the server, try it now and get help in the lab if you need it. Here’s what my public_html/online_experiments_practicals/ folder currently looks like on cyberduck.

suggested directory structure

If your directory structure is the same as mine the url for your experiment will be https://jspsychlearning.ppls.ed.ac.uk/~UUN/online_experiments_practicals/word_learning/my_word_learning.html where UUN is your UUN. If you want to see what the finished experiment will look like, you can run the word_learning.html file which includes our final implementation - the URL will be https://jspsychlearning.ppls.ed.ac.uk/~UUN/online_experiments_practicals/word_learning/word_learning.html, or you can run my copy on jspsychlearning. Remember that if you look at the data display at the end that will include spoilers on how to write the code yourself, so depending on how much you want to challenge yourself you might want to avoid clicking through to the very end!

If you run through our implementation of the experiment you’ll see that, in addition to the usual instructions, the experiment consists of two stages: observation and production. Observation trials involve presentation of an object plus a label; production trials prompt the participant to select a label for an object. In a little more detail:

(NB. Ferdinand et al. have a 3rd stage at the end of each production trial where you then see the object plus the selected label for 2 further seconds - I have not included that in the code here, I don’t think it’s crucial and I couldn’t make it look nice in jsPsych without making everything else much more complicated!).

To implement your own observation and (simplified) production trials, you need to figure out what plugin(s) you need for observation and production trials (think about the stimulus and the response), then look at the documentation for that plugin to see how to use it and/or edit previous bits of code you have written that do something similar. The novel-object images you need are in the folder images. Your code should go in my_word_learning.html and my_word_learning.js.

Remember, it’s OK to implement this in a very simple way, with a long manually-constructed trial list (which is probably a good way to start) - but if you feel more adventurous and want to use some of the ideas that were introduced last week (nested timelines, even functions to build trials) that’s great too. Give it a go, see how you get on, and ask for help if/when you get stuck!

Walk-through of our implementation

As you hopefully figured out when you were trying to build it yourself, the main part of the experiment uses a plugin you may not have used before (image-button-response), but you have used other image plugins (e.g. in the “hello world” exercise you used image-keyboard-response to display an image), and you have seen lots of button-response plugins (e.g. html-button-response for instruction screens). Our implementation uses those plugins, but we are going to add some extra machinery to randomise the placement of choices on-screen, and to allow the choices on one trial to depend on choices made at the previous trial (to get the click-to-confirm behaviour in production trials).

Nested timelines again

Like in the self-paced reading experiment, individual trials in this word-learning experiment are somewhat complex - they involve a couple of steps. Complex multi-part trials should sound familiar from the self-paced reading experiment, and we are going to handle it in the same way, by using nested timelines - the only difference is that each trial in the nested timeline in the self-paced reading experiment was essentially the same (see a word, press space) whereas here the component trials differ a little more. But in our implementation we are also going to do some stuff with data property of each trial, so we’ll start there.

Trial data

You should by now be familiar with the idea that each jsPsych trial has some properties that we can set - the trial type (html with keyboard response, image with button response etc), the valid choices, the trial_duration etc. In the same way, each trial has a data property. By default the data property is populated automatically by the plugin, and records data relevant to that trial type - for each plugin you’ll notice there’s a section of the documentation telling you what it records, for instance I can see from the image-button-response documentation (which is the plugin that we’ll be using in this experiment) that it records reaction time and the index of the button that the participant pressed. But we are also allowed to add stuff to the data property, to augment this automatically-generated content.

In this experiment we are going to use this data property in two ways. First, we are going to flag trials which actually contain important data. You will have already noticed that jsPsych gathers data on all trial types, including things like reaction times and stimulus on the consent and information screens. Recording everything is a good way to avoid losing anything, but it does make for quite a cluttered data structure at the end of the experiment. For certain critical trials in this experiment, we are going to add some information to the trial data, a block property, indicating trials that belong to the experiment phases/blocks that we really care about (what the participant saw on an observation trial, what they selected on a production trial); marking up those trials in that way will make it easy to find the important data when you are analysing your data.

Second, the data from one trial sticks around as the rest of the experiment runs. We can therefore look at the data property of earlier trials when constructing a new trial, which allows us to build sequences of trials where what the participant does at one trial (e.g. which button they clicked) affects what they see at the next trial: we look at the data from the earlier trial, extract the info we want, then use that to build the new trial.

Observation trials

OK, let’s get started with the code. Remember that each observation trial consists of 2 steps: display the object (an image) for 1 second, then display the object plus a label (some text) for 2 seconds. There are several ways you could do this in jsPsych, most obviously using the image-keyboard-response or image-button-response plugins - since we will need buttons later, I am going to use the image-button-response plugin.

Again, the simplest way to do this would be to construct each sub-part of each observation trial as a stand-alone trial, and then stick them together into a simple flat timeline. For instance, if I want to show object4 (a shiny cylinder thing) paired with the labels ‘buv’ and ‘cal’ several times I could do something like this:

var observation_object4_only = {
  type: jsPsychImageButtonResponse,
  stimulus: "images/object4.jpg",
  choices: [],
  trial_duration: 1000,
};

var observation_object4_buv = {
  type: jsPsychImageButtonResponse,
  stimulus: "images/object4.jpg",
  choices: [],
  prompt: "buv",
  trial_duration: 2000,
};

var observation_object4_cal = {
  type: jsPsychImageButtonResponse,
  stimulus: "images/object4.jpg",
  choices: [],
  prompt: "cal",
  trial_duration: 2000,
};

var simple_observation_trials = [
  observation_object4_only,
  observation_object4_buv,
  observation_object4_only,
  observation_object4_cal,
  observation_object4_only,
  observation_object4_buv,
  observation_object4_only,
  observation_object4_buv,
  observation_object4_only,
  observation_object4_cal,
];

Then if we slot that simple_observation_trials into our timeline we will get the trial sequence we want.

A couple of things to note here.

This approach would work OK, but it has a couple of drawbacks. Firstly, the fact that the observation_object4_only trial doesn’t have a prompt means that things will jump about a bit on the screen - the image will move up when the experiment reaches the trials with labels, to make space for the prompt, then drop down again when we are showing the object with no label, and all that movement is quite unpleasant to look at for the participant. This is actually easily fixed by including some dummy text as a prompt on the trials where we don’t want any text in the prompt - then every trial has a prompt, and so things don’t jump around on-screen so much. We could do that like this, using   which is a special whitespace character in HTML that will give us a blank prompt:

var observation_object4_only = {
  type: jsPsychImageButtonResponse,
  stimulus: "images/object4.jpg",
  choices: [],
  prompt: " ", //dummy text
  trial_duration: 1000,
};

Note that just including prompt: " " doesn’t work, we have to include some content there.

The more important problem with this simple approach, like I said in connection with the self-paced reading experiment, is that building this flat timeline is going to be very laborious and redundant for an experiment involving more than a few observation trials, and quite error prone (even just writing out this little example I forgot to change the prompt for the second trial from “buv” to “cal”, which would be an important mistake in a frequency-learning experiment), and there is no easy way to randomise the trial list without hopelessly scrambling everything.

So instead I am adopting the same approach as in the self-paced reading experiment: using nested timelines to tie together the sub-parts of a single trial, and wrapping the whole thing in a function that builds a single observation trial for us in a neat, compartmentalised way. The code for that is as follows:

function make_observation_trial(object, label) {
  var object_filename = "images/" + object + ".jpg"; //build file name for the object
  trial = {
    type: jsPsychImageButtonResponse,
    stimulus: object_filename,
    choices: [],
    timeline: [
      {
        prompt: " ", //dummy text
        trial_duration: 1000,
      },
      { prompt: label, 
        trial_duration: 2000, 
        data: { block: "observation" } },
    ],
  };
  return trial;
};

This bit of code creates a function, called make_observation_trial. We specify the object and the label and it does the rest for us, returning a complex trial with a nested timeline containing the two sub-parts (object only, then object plus label).

A couple of things to note here:

Now we can use this function to make some observation trials - in the code I make a 5-trial observation phase, where object4 is paired with two non-word labels, “buv” and “cal”. The first step is to make those two trial types with the two different labels, using our new function:

var observation_trial_object4_buv = make_observation_trial("object4", "buv");
var observation_trial_object4_cal = make_observation_trial("object4", "cal");

Now we are going to need several of these trials in training - let’s say I want 3 buvs and 2 cals. I could just do this manually, but it’s easier and less error-prone to use the built-in function that jsPsych provides for repeating trials, jsPsych.randomization.repeat.

var observation_trials = jsPsych.randomization.repeat(
  [observation_trial_object4_buv, observation_trial_object4_cal],
  [3, 2]
);

Note that we give jsPsych.randomization.repeat a list of trials that we want repeated, and a second list telling it how many repetitions we want of each of those trials (3 of the first one, 2 of the second). The documentation for that repeat function is here if you are curious.

And that’s our observation timeline built. Now we need to build the production trials.

Production trials

We will use some of the same tricks (a function that creates a trial with a nested timeline, adding a block property to the data so we can spot the production trials easily later), but also some new stuff to handle contingent trials.

Remember that each production trial consists of two steps: display the object plus two labels and have the participant select a label, then have the participant confirm their label choice with a further click in the middle. Step 1 is fairly straightforward, except that we want to copy Ferdinand et al. and randomise the left-right order in which the labels appear on each trial (this would also have come in handy last week if you were working on the optional maze task exercise). But step 2 is tricky - the label shown at the 2nd step of the trial needs to depend on what button the participant clicks on the 1st step.

Rather than dumping the final code in here I am going to talk you through it in the same way as for the observation phase, starting out with imagining how you’d do a single production trial as a sequence of two separate trials, then going from that to a single trial with a nested timeline.

Here’s a simple way to implement the 1st step of a production trial - show the object plus two labelled buttons. Let’s say we want to show object4 with the options buv and cal, to follow on from our observation phase above.

var production_step1 = {
  type: jsPsychImageButtonResponse,
  stimulus: "images/object4.jpg",
  choices: ["buv", "cal"],
};

That is very simple, but the labels will always appear in the same order - buv on the left, cal on the right. That might be a problem - maybe people will be biased to click on one side, or maybe this will encourage them to always click on the same side and given very self-consistent responses just because they are being lazy. So we want to randomise the order of the buttons, and we want to do this independently for every trial, so that sometimes buv is on the left and sometimes it’s on the right.

There are a couple of ways you could do this in jsPsych. I am going to do it using the on_start property of trials. This allows us to specify some code to run when the trial starts but before anything is displayed on screen, and importantly the stuff that happens in on_start can alter the other trial properties. Specifically, initially we’ll start off with empty choices (it will complain if we try to leave choices unspecified, so we have to set it to something) and then generate a random ordering of the labels in the on_start.

var production_step1 = {
  type: jsPsychImageButtonResponse,
  stimulus: "images/object4.jpg",
  choices: [], //dummy choices initially
  on_start: function (trial) {
    var shuffled_labels = jsPsych.randomization.shuffle(["buv", "cal"]);
    trial.choices = shuffled_labels;
  },
};

Inside on_start we shuffle the two labels, using another randomisation function provided by jsPsych, jsPsych.randomization.shuffle, which will randomise the order of items in a list we give it. We can then set the trial’s choices parameter to that shuffled ordering (overwriting the dummy choices we started with) with the code

trial.choices = shuffled_labels;

So by the time the participant actually sees the choices on the screen, on_start will already have done its work and the two buttons will appear in a randomised order.

That will work, but we still haven’t addressed the trickiest problem - how do we build the 2nd step of a production trial, where the label I select at step 1 is shown to me again for confirmation / to center my mouse? This is a pretty common thing to want to do - there are many experimental designs where you want to make behaviour at later trials depend on the participant’s response, for example you might want to provide corrective feedback, repeat trials that a participant gets wrong, or (as in our case) show something that relates to their earlier response.

The way to do this is to store the info you need from one trial in its data property, then later on you can use some built-in jsPsych functions to look back at the earlier trial and read the information you need from the relevant bit of that data. We already know that button response trials automatically record the index of the button the participant pressed, in data.response - that will be 0 if they pressed the first button, 1 if they pressed the second, etc. But that actually isn’t super-useful, because we are randomising the button positions - we don’t know if button 0 is buv or cal in our example, and (slightly weirdly in my opinion), jsPsych doesn’t automatically record the choices parameter to data. But it does provide a way for us to specify that the trial choices should be added to the data, so we will use that, and then on the next trial we can dig it out and use it. At the start of the step 1 trial we’ll make a note of the order of the randomised labels (using the trial parameter save_trial_parameters). Then after the participant has made their selection (in on_finish), we’ll use our knowledge of the order the buttons appeared and the info on which button they pressed to work out which label they selected, and record that in data too. Then in step 2 we can just retrieve that information. So our step 1 trial would look like this:

var production_step1 = {
  type: jsPsychImageButtonResponse,
  stimulus: "images/object4.jpg",
  choices: [], //dummy choices initially
  data: { block: "production" }, //add a note that this is a production trial
  save_trial_parameters: {choices: true}, //and we want to save the trial choices
  on_start: function (trial) {
    var shuffled_labels = jsPsych.randomization.shuffle(["buv", "cal"]);
    trial.choices = shuffled_labels;
  },
  on_finish: function (data) {
    var button_number = data.response;
    data.label_selected = data.choices[button_number];
  },
};

save_trial_parameters: {choices: true} is the instruction to save the trial choices into the trial data - if you wanted you could save additional trial parameters (e.g. the prompt, if you cared!) in the same way. Then we add an on_finish parameter, which looks up which button the participant pressed (data.response - the plugin records that automatically for us) and combines that with the data.choices info we saved to work out what label they selected (data.choices[button_number] will return the 0th label in choices if they clicked button 0, the 1st label if they clicked button 1, etc) and save that info in the trial data too, as data.label_selected.

Then the second step of the trial is fairly straightforward - when that trial starts (i.e. using on_start again) we can use a built-in jsPsych function to retrieve the data from the previous trial, then just read off the label_selected info we saved. That looks like this:

var production_step2 = {
  type: jsPsychImageButtonResponse,
  stimulus: "images/object4.jpg",
  choices: [], //dummy choices initially
  on_start: function (trial) {
    var last_trial_data = jsPsych.data.get().last(1).values()[0];
    var last_trial_label = last_trial_data.label_selected;
    trial.choices = [last_trial_label];
  },
};

The only slightly intimidating part of that is the first line where we use jsPsych.data.get().last(1).values()[0] to access the last trial. jsPsych.data.get() is a jsPsych function that returns all the data from all trials so far, so we have to dig into it to get the last trial; that’s what last(1) does - if you wanted to get the last 5 trials you could do that with e.g. last(5). So that gives us the last trial, but that contains a lot of info we don’t need so we dig out what we want using the values() function (I have no idea what all the other stuff saved there is to be honest), then that gives us a list of which we take the first item (which is what the [0] does), and at last we have our data from the last trial. In case you are wondering how I figured all that out: I didn’t, it’s in the Dynamic parameters section of the jsPsych overview, I just copied it and worked from there. Anyway, once we have our last trial data we just retrieve the info we want (which we saved under label_selected), then we set the choices for this trial to that label and we are done. Phew.

Or nearly done. Of course doing every production trial as a sequence of 2 trials would be a pain, for all the usual reasons, so instead what we are going to do is wrap those two component trials up in a function that creates a complex trial with a nested timeline. But all the logic and the details are the same - we give the function the image and the label choices, and it builds us a complex trial. That’s what is in the code below. I have made one tiny addition, which is to add some block information to the trial data for the crucial click-a-button part of this trial, just like I added block information to the observation trials above - this time I note that this is a production trial rather than an observation trial.

function make_production_trial(object, labels) {
  var object_filename = "images/" + object + ".jpg";
  var trial = {
    type: jsPsychImageButtonResponse,
    stimulus: object_filename,
    timeline: [
      //subtrial 1: show the two labelled buttons and have the participant select
      {
        choices: [], //dummy choices to be over-written on_start
        //at the start of the trial, randomise the left-right order of the labels
        //and note that randomisation in data as label_choices
        on_start: function (trial) {
          var shuffled_labels =
            jsPsych.randomization.shuffle(labels);
          trial.choices = shuffled_labels;
          trial.data = {
            block: "production",
            label_choices: shuffled_labels,
          };
        },
        //at the end of the trial, use data.response to figure out
        //which label they selected, and add that to data
        on_finish: function (data) {
          var button_number = data.response;
          data.label_selected = data.label_choices[button_number];
        },
      },
      //subtrial 2: show the image plus selected label, make the participant click that label
      //(to re-center their mouse)
      {
        choices: [], //dummy choices to be over-written on_start
        on_start: function (trial) {
          //get the last trial response (the data generated by the button-click)
          var last_trial_data = jsPsych.data.get().last(1).values()[0];
          //look up the label_selected on that last trial
          var last_trial_label = last_trial_data.label_selected;
          trial.choices = [last_trial_label]; //this is your only choice
        },
      },
    ],
  };
  return trial;
};

That is a fairly scary-looking bit of code, but hopefully you understand how the two sub-trials fit together now you have seen it built from scratch. If not, ask in labs!

At long last we can build our list of production trials using this function - I’ll take 5 trials, to test participants 5 times on the label for object 4, which I can also do with jsPsych.randomization.repeat.

var production_trial_object4 = make_production_trial('object4',['buv','cal']);
var production_trials = jsPsych.randomization.repeat([production_trial_object4], 5);

Building a timeline using concat

The next bit of the code is the usual stuff with placeholders for consent and instructions, so I’ll skip over that. The very final few lines of the code then build and run our timeline.

For this experiment, when we build the timeline we use a javascript function called concat, which will generate a nice flat timeline of the sort jsPsych expects.

var full_timeline = [].concat(
  consent_screen,
  instruction_screen_observation,
  observation_trials,
  instruction_screen_production,
  production_trials,
  final_screen
);

What that essentially says is “take an empty array ([]) and then concatenate (add) to it whatever is in the variables consent_screen instruction_screen_observation, observation_trials, etc.

You might be wondering why we can’t do something like this, which has worked in the past:

var full_timeline = 
[
  consent_screen,
  instruction_screen_observation,
  observation_trials,
  instruction_screen_production,
  production_trials,
  final_screen
];

The problem is that our variables we are sticking together includes a mix of individual trials (e.g. consent_screen) and lists of trials (e.g. observation_trials). That is going to confuse jsPsych - it wants everything in the experiment timeline to be trial, so it can consult its type property and know what to do, and it doesn’t know what to do when it gets to a list of trials, so it throws an error and breaks. We can get round this by using concat, which allows us to stitch together individual trials and lists of trials in a way that makes our life easy but keeps jsPsych happy.

Saving data

As usual, when we finish (so using the on_finish parameter of initJsPsych, right at the top of the code) we are going to display the data on the screen. But we also want to save the data to a csv file on the server. There are three new lines of code to do that.

var jsPsych = initJsPsych({
  on_finish: function () {
    var all_data = jsPsych.data.get(); //get all data
    var all_data_as_csv = all_data.csv(); //convert to csv format
    save_data("wordlearning_data.csv", all_data_as_csv); //save it
    jsPsych.data.displayData("csv"); //and also dump the data to screen
  },
});

That uses two jsPsych functions to get the trial data, concert it to CSV format, then finally we use the save_data function (copied directly from section 06 of Alisdair’s tutorial code) to save that data to the server in a file called wordlearning_data.csv - if you run the code on the jspsychlearning server you should see there is a file called wordlearning_data.csv in the folder called server_data, which is at quite a high level in your directory structure (you might have to jump up a few levels in the directory structure to find that folder). Note also that cyberduck doesn’t automatically refresh the list of files when a new file is added, so you may have to click the “Refresh” button to see your data file. Every time you run the code it will append more data to that file, so if you have run the code a few times it might look quite messy! The save_data function isn’t actually included in word_learning.js - I have saved it in a separate utilities.js file, which is loaded in word_learning.html:

        <script src="utilities.js"></script>
        <script src="word_learning.js"></script>

Since it’s a function we use with every experiment and it’s basically always the same it makes sense to me to separate it off and save it in a file I can treat as a black box and forget about.

Exercises with the word learning experiment code

Attempt these problems. After the practical you will be able to consult some notes on the answers

References

Ferdinand, V., Kirby, S., & Smith, K. (2019). The cognitive roots of regularization in language. Cognition, 184, 53-68.

Re-use

All aspects of this work are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.


Course main page

XXProject maintained by kennysmithed Hosted on GitHub Pages — Theme by mattgraham